Skip to main content

Gambling on a fallacy

Probability is at the heart of much of life - yet it can seem counter-intuitive. One of the best-known aspects of this is the so-called gambler's fallacy (and its inverted equivalent, the hot hand fallacy). 

Take a simple example of tossing a coin repeatedly. A head comes up ten times in a row. Now we all know that, on average and over time, a fair coin should give us the same percentage of heads and tails. So it can seem surprisingly reasonable that the next throw is more likely to be a tail. 

That's the gambler's fallacy, because, of course, the coin has no memory. It has no way of telling what came before, so the next toss is still equally likely to be heads or tails. (Some argue that actually it's more likely to come up a head again - for a demonstration of why this can be the case, see the start of my video below.)

At a simple level, it's quite easy to get your head around what's happening here, but some of the subtleties of the gambler's fallacy are still easy to miss - and that's where a new paper by Steven Tijms comes in (you can read the full thing here). Tijms points out how counter-intuitive the behaviour of randomness is. For example, he shows how to calculate the chances of getting, say, five or more heads or tails in a row in 100 coin tosses - the answer is remarkably 97.2%. Even a run of seven or more with the same face is more likely to occur than not in 100.

We also see how the law of large numbers - the idea that, for example, the ratio of heads to tails will get closer to 50:50 as we repeat the toss many times - doesn't work the way we tend to assume it will. In part this is because we underestimate what 'many times' means. Tijms points out, for example, to be 95% sure that the relative frequencies of heads and tails is in the 48-52% range it's necessary to toss a coin around 2,500 times. But also there's something more fundamental that fuels the gambler's fallacy.

Because the relative frequencies are tending towards 50:50 as we get more and more, it's tempting to think that the gap between number of heads and number of tails thrown must reduce. And so, if there's a big gap, say many more heads than tails, it's hard to resist the feeling that the coin needs to counter this by coming up with a lot more tails. But this is still the gambler's fallacy. It's perfectly possible for those relatively frequencies to get closer and closer to 50:50 yet to have the gap between numbers of heads and tails increase rather than decrease.

I've only been able to give a taster of Tijm's paper - I'd recommend taking a look as it is very readable and is not loaded with scary mathematics.

See all of Brian's online articles or subscribe to a weekly digest for free here

Here's the video for the coin toss oddity:

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Why I hate opera

If I'm honest, the title of this post is an exaggeration to make a point. I don't really hate opera. There are a couple of operas - notably Monteverdi's Incoranazione di Poppea and Purcell's Dido & Aeneas - that I quite like. But what I do find truly sickening is the reverence with which opera is treated, as if it were some particularly great art form. Nowhere was this more obvious than in ITV's 2010 gut-wrenchingly awful series Pop Star to Opera Star , where the likes of Alan Tichmarsh treated the real opera singers as if they were fragile pieces on Antiques Roadshow, and the music as if it were a gift of the gods. In my opinion - and I know not everyone agrees - opera is: Mediocre music Melodramatic plots Amateurishly hammy acting A forced and unpleasant singing style Ridiculously over-supported by public funds I won't even bother to go into any detail on the plots and the acting - this is just self-evident. But the other aspects need some exp...

Murder by Candlelight - Ed. Cecily Gayford ***

Nothing seems to suit Christmas reading better than either ghost stories or Christmas-set novels. For some this means a fluffy romance in the snow, but for those of us with darker preferences, it's hard to beat a good Christmas murder. An annual event for me over the last few years has been getting the excellent series of classic murderous Christmas short stories pulled together by Cecily Gayford, starting with the 2016 Murder under the Christmas Tree . This featured seasonal output from the likes of Margery Allingham, Arthur Conan Doyle, Ellis Peters and Dorothy L. Sayers, laced with a few more modern authors such as Ian Rankin and Val McDermid, in some shiny Christmassy twisty tales. I actually thought while purchasing this year's addition 'Surely she is going to run out of classic stories soon' - and sadly, to a degree, Gayford has. The first half of Murder by Candlelight is up to the usual standard with some good seasonal tales from the likes of Catherine Aird, Car...

Why backgammon is a better game than chess

I freely admit that chess, for those who enjoy it, is a wonderful game, but I honestly believe that as a game , backgammon is better (and this isn't just because I'm a lot better at playing backgammon than chess). Having relatively recently written a book on game theory, I have given quite a lot of thought to the nature of games, and from that I'd say that chess has two significant weaknesses compared with backgammon. One is the lack of randomness. Because backgammon includes the roll of the dice, it introduces a random factor into the play. Of course, a game that is totally random provides very little enjoyment. Tossing a coin isn't at all entertaining. But the clever thing about backgammon is that the randomness is contributory without dominating - there is still plenty of room for skill (apart from very flukey dice throws, I can always be beaten by a really good backgammon player), but the introduction of a random factor makes it more life-like, with more of a sense...